Painted Rock: a natural alignment with Chumash cosmology

Featured

In the heart of California’s Carrizo Plain National Monument, an ancient natural formation known as Painted Rock stands as a testament to the region’s rich cultural and ecological history. For centuries, the Chumash people painted many petroglyphs on it inside walls and regarded this site as a sacred place, using it for ceremonies, initiations, and astronomical observations. Recent observations reveal an extraordinary feature of Painted Rock: its alignment with key celestial events, echoing the cosmology of the Chumash in remarkable ways.

The Chumash were a highly advanced and vibrant society whose territory stretched from Malibu Beach in the south to the central regions of San Luis Obispo County in the north, bordered by the Carrizo Plain and Mount Piños to the east (the highest peak in the region at 8,831 feet), and the Channel Islands to the west. Archaeological evidence shows that the Chumash settled in the Carrizo Plain as early as 2000 BCE, with their population peaking around 1000 CE. However, the population diminished steeply after that, likely due to significant climate changes that affected the region’s resources, Some Peoples from the Yokuts settled and shared the area until the 19th century.

The Carrizo Plain is bordered on the east by the San Andreas Fault, a geographical feature that not only shaped the landscape but also influenced the water sources and ecosystems the Chumash depended upon. Painted Rock, located near the heart of the plain, became a spiritual and cultural focal point for the Chumash people.

Chumash cosmology divided the universe into three interconnected realms: the underworld, the middle world, and the sky world. Humans inhabited the middle world and were tasked with maintaining balance between the realms, which were permeated by a neutral energy. Unlike many cultures that revered the Sun, the Chumash held the North Star (Polaris) as the most significant celestial object, symbolizing stability and balance in the universe.

A map of the Carrizo Plain National Monument. You can see the location of Painted Rock on the center right.

“California’s Chumash Indians thought of the sky gods this way. They saw a balance of nature and the world order in terms of a nightly gambling game played between two teams. Sun was the captain of one team, while the pole star, Polaris, led the other. Polaris was known as Sky Coyote, and its pivotal position among the stars made it a symbol of the night.

For a full year they played, and at the winter solstice the score was tallied. Moon, that expert at counting out the days, kept score. If, at the winter solstice, Sun was the winner, it would go bad for people on earth: rather than return upon his yearly journey back to the north, he might just continue on south and leave the earth in the dead of winter, with the cosmos out of balance. Sky Coyote was a benefactor, a benevolent influence. If his team won, the order of things would be restored.” E.C. Krupp Echoes of the Ancient Skies

A Petroglyph of the Sky Coyote on the inside of Painted Rock (unfortunately damaged like many of the other painting on the rock ) photo by Dan McCaslin / Noozhawk

My wife, Sarah Pillow, and I have been visiting the Carrizo Plain for many years, drawn by its unspoiled landscapes, pristine night skies, and rich history. Our connection to the area deepened through stargazing and learning about the indigenous Chumash culture, so vividly demonstrated at the Painted Rock monument. As an amateur astronomer with a passion for sky lore and archeo-astronomy, I was particularly intrigued by how the Chumash integrated celestial events into their cultural practices.

In August 2023, I conducted a detailed scan of the interior of Painted Rock using an iPhone and the Scaniverse app. The scan data was assembled in Blender, and I also created a 360˚ panoramic photo of the site, which I inserted into the Stellarium app for astronomical analysis. The results were striking: the rock formation, shaped like a large horseshoe, has its opening perfectly oriented towards Polaris, a celestial body of profound significance in Chumash cosmology. (see below a short video that show the North view of a whole year motion of the constelation around Polaris)

whole year motion of the constelation around Polaris

Another remarkable discovery emerged during my analysis: on the south side of Painted Rock, the Sun at noon during the winter solstice aligns precisely with the top of the formation. This natural alignment mirrors the Chumash’s understanding of celestial cycles, which informed their rituals and worldview. (see below a short video of the Sun, during a whole year at noon, looking south from inside the rock)

video of the Sun, during a whole year at noon, looking south from inside the rock

What makes this discovery particularly fascinating is that Painted Rock is a natural formation, not a human-made structure. Its alignment with these celestial events appears to be a fortuitous occurrence, yet it seamlessly supports the Chumash belief system. It highlights the deep connection the Chumash people saw between their sacred geography and the cosmos.

An aerial of Painted Rock

Tragically, the Chumash population was devastated during colonization. The establishment of Spanish missions introduced disease, forced labor, and cultural disruption, and westward expansion by settlers led to further extermination of their population and heritage. Yet, the Chumash legacy endures through their artifacts, traditions, and the profound cosmological insights that continue to inspire and amaze.

Part of a large painting on the inner wall of Painted Rock, unfortunately vandalized modern inhabitant of the area (the location is now fenced in and you need to register to visit it)

The Carrizo Plain is more than just the home of Painted Rock. It is a vital ecological and cultural preserve, with vibrant wildflower blooms, rare wildlife, and one of California’s last native grasslands. Its dark, unpolluted skies make it an ideal destination for stargazers and those seeking to connect with the celestial traditions of the past.

I hope this discovery inspires further exploration of Painted Rock and its connections to the Chumash worldview. By uncovering and honoring the interplay between culture, nature, and astronomy, we can better appreciate the profound legacy of the Chumash and their relationship with the universe. If you are fortunate enough to visit the Carrizo Plain, approach Painted Rock with reverence, keeping in mind its cultural and spiritual significance. Together, we can ensure this sacred site remains a source of wonder and learning for generations to come.

MUSIC COMMUNALITIES AND DIFFERENCES: SOCIOLOGY THROUGH SOUND

Featured

How do different cultures hear consonances; what are the facts that affect our perceptions and how might a word we use change if it is spoken or sung?


Is the way we hear and sing music unique to all ethnicities? And why, as we become aware by listening to music from around the world, do these differences and similarities arise? What follows is a summarization of two studies on this topic.

1. Communalities (consonances)

In the first study, the art of music is universal: comparative studies made on the subject show that there are underlining patterns that exist across cultures. In almost all of them, we find that the pitch of spoken and sung words have several different behaviors. For example, sung words are produced at a higher pitch; the pace of the word is slower; and the pitch is more stable than when the words are spoken.


In a paper on anthropology in Science Advances, the author put these facts to the test:
https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/sciadv.adm9797


Concerning the pitch of a word, the article states: “we realized that in speech we recognize phonemes by the shape of formants (formants are an analysis of the component frequencies that make up human speech). These formants characterize how upper harmonics’ content is emphasized or attenuated. In speech, the frequency content that is conveying information is not fundamental frequency but harmonics; whereas in music, it is the lower fundamental frequencies that contain the crucial melodic content. We speculate that the difference in emphasis on formants versus fundamental frequency may underlie the difference in pitch height between speech and music we have identified.”


The “use of longer acoustic phrases, greater sound pressure, and less noisy sounds may ease the intelligibility of pitch information. This increased loudness and salience might also support evolutionary propositions that music evolved as a mnemonic device or as a night-time, long-distance communication device. The lyrics of the chosen songs [of the study] frequently mention “night,” “moon,” “sleep,” and “love,” which may further support the nocturnal hypothesis.”

Here’s a word cloud that illustrates this point:


To recapitulate features that the study identified as differentiating music and speech along a musi-linguistic continuum are pitch height, temporal rate, and pitch stability.
Since utilization of pitch can also be found in language (e.g., tonal languages: increasing the pitch of the final word in an interrogative sentence in today’s English and Japanese), inclusively probing what we can communicate with pitch in human acoustic communication may give insights into the fundamental nature of songs.


Meanwhile, the features identified as shared between speech and song—particularly timbral brightness and pitch interval size— represent promising candidates for understanding the role of vocalization that may shape the cultural evolution of music and language. Together, these cross-cultural similarities and differences may help shed light on the cultural and biological evolution of two systems that make us human: music and language.

2. Differences (dissonances)

The debate about following perfect mathematical rules, which was supported by all the followers of the strict tuning rules established by Pythagoras and based on the natural harmonic series, started to be questioned by the end of the Renaissance. The musicians of the time became more inclined to follow their ears and bend the strict rules to attain smoother relationships between the notes of the scale. In any case, these strict rules were never completely followed, as is clear when listening and comparing Western to Chinese and South Asian music. This study is addressing how these different tunings came to be used around the world.


The complete article: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-024-45812-z


This study explored harmonic relationships that are pleasant or unpleasant to our ears by having participants listen to “dyads” (two-note intervals) in which the sounds were manipulated in several ways.


First – by compressing or stretching the harmonic partials as shown below in order to learn how these manipulations affected the perception of pleasantness of the listeners:

Compressed and streched harmonic partial

Intuitively, this can be understood from the observation that interference is minimized when partials from different tones align neatly with each other (as in the harmonic sequence above). If we then stretch each tone’s spectrum, we must also stretch the intervals between the tones to maintain this alignment, meaning that the fundamental will stray from pure Pythagorean harmonics. Once the individual tones become inharmonic, the overall chord also becomes inharmonic, irrespective of the intervals between the tones.


The result therefore provides evidence that interference between partials is an important contributor to consonance perception.


The metallophones of Indonesian gamelans and the xylophone-like renats used in Thai classical music – each instrument has an idiosyncratic spectrum that reflects its particular physical construction, with potentially interesting implications for consonance perception. The study investigated an inharmonic tone inspired by one such instrument, the bonang, which uses the slendro scale.


The inharmonic slendro scale might be explained in terms of the consonance profile produced by combining a harmonic complex tone with a bonang tone.


The stretching/compressing manipulation is interesting from a modeling perspective, because it clearly dissociates the predictions of the interference and the harmonicity models (partials in defining pleasantness and unpleasantness is called “harmonicity”). It shows that only the former are compatible with the resultant scales. The latter manipulation is interesting from a cultural evolution perspective, because it supports the hypothesis that the slendro scale developed in part as a specific consequence of the acoustic properties of Javanese gamelan instruments.


The study found that manipulating the frequencies of the harmonics can induce inharmonic consonance profiles. If we stretch or compress this partial, we also have to stretch/compress the distance between the two tones in order to hear a pleasant harmony.


The second step is to enhance or cancel the harmonics in order to understand the role that these harmonics have in our judgment. First, the study considered how consonance profiles may be affected by changing the amplitudes of their tones’ harmonics. In particular, we focus on the so-called spectral roll-off parameter, which determines the rate at which harmonic amplitude rolls off (decreases) as harmonic numbers increase, by increasing the harmonic component:

The study determined no clear effect on pleasantness variability; the profiles remain highly differentiated for all roll-off levels.


Finally, they tested whether consonance profiles are affected by a more radical manipulation by completely deleting particular harmonics:

Deleted selective harmonics

Removing the harmonicity of the tones in fact reduces the straying away of the pitches from natural harmony.


In summary, the results point to an important contribution of harmonicity to consonance perception. It becomes clear that the upper harmonics of a tone has an important role in defining pleasantness in dyad intervals.


These results provide an empirical foundation for the idea that cultural variation in scale systems might in part be driven by the spectral properties of the musical instruments used by these different cultures. In Western culture, when the reference point is generally based on string instruments, the harmonic tone relationships are different than the ones from South East Asia. In Java, for example, tone relationships are based on Bonang – a collection of small gongs from the Gamelan ensemble, which have a very different series of harmonics or harmonicity, therefore ending up with very different scales used by both Western and South Asian cultures.


The Javanese slendro scale and the pelog scale, both of which are pentatonic, deviate considerably from the Western 12-tone scale.


On the other hand, it is interesting to know that this differentiation is not anthropomorphic: a Westerner listening to Bonang instruments would come to the same conclusion as a South East Asian listener regarding the pleasantness of an interval, and vice-versa. It is indeed the instrument and their specific harmonicity that defines the scale chosen.


So there you have it – the first study found universal relationships between song and spoken words; the second found a locality in the judging of pleasantness in harmonic relationships between tones, although this difference is environmental, not fundamental.


I hope that this will help us understand that in the end, we are the same and our similarities are more than our differences.